Slime
Molds Remember — but Do They Learn?
Evidence mounts that organisms
without nervous systems can in some sense learn and solve problems, but
researchers disagree about whether this is “primitive cognition.”
https://www.quantamagazine.org/slime-molds-remember-but-do-they-learn-20180709/?utm_source=Quanta+Magazine&utm_campaign=2ecb15ab76-RSS_Daily_Biology&utm_medium=email&utm_term=0_f0cb61321c-2ecb15ab76-389846569&mc_cid=2ecb15ab76&mc_eid=61275b7d81
[[Absolutely fascinating, and
shows how little we understand.]]
Despite its single-celled
simplicity and lack of a nervous system, the slime mold Physarum
polycephalum may be capable of an elementary form of learning,
according to some suggestive experimental results.
Audrey
Dussutour, CNRS
July
9, 2018
Slime
molds are among the world’s strangest organisms. Long mistaken for fungi, they
are now classed as a type of amoeba. As single-celled organisms, they have
neither neurons nor brains. Yet for about a decade, scientists have debated
whether slime molds have the capacity to learn about their environments and
adjust their behavior accordingly.
For Audrey Dussutour, a biologist at France’s
National Center for Scientific Research and a team leader at the Research
Center on Animal Cognition at Université Paul Sabatier in Toulouse, that debate
is over. Her group not only taught slime molds to ignore noxious substances
that they would normally avoid, but demonstrated that the organisms could
remember this behavior after a year of physiologically disruptive enforced
sleep. But do these results prove that slime molds — and perhaps a wide range of
other organisms that lack brains — can exhibit a form of primitive cognition?
Slime molds are relatively easy
to study, as protozoa go. They are macroscopic organisms that can be easily
manipulated and observed. There are more than 900 species of slime mold; some
live as single-celled organisms most of the time, but come together in a swarm
to forage and procreate when food is short. Others, so-called plasmodial slime
molds, always live as one huge cell containing thousands of nuclei. Most
importantly, slime molds can be taught new tricks; depending on the species,
they may not like caffeine, salt or strong light, but they can learn that no-go
areas marked with these are not as bad as they seem, a process known as
habituation.
“By classical definitions of habituation,
this primitive unicellular organism is learning, just as animals with brains
do,” said Chris Reid, a behavioral
biologist at Macquarie University in Australia. “As slime molds don’t have any
neurons, the mechanisms of the learning process must be completely different;
however, the outcome and functional significance are the same.”
For Dussutour, “that such
organisms have the capacity to learn has considerable implications beyond
recognizing learning in nonneural systems.” She believes that slime molds may
help scientists to understand when and where in the tree of life the earliest
manifestations of learning evolved.
Even more intriguingly, and
perhaps controversially, research by Dussutour and others suggests that slime
molds can transfer their acquired memories from cell to cell, said František Baluška, a plant
cell biologist at the University of Bonn. “This is extremely exciting for our
understanding of much larger organisms such as animals, humans and plants.”
A History of Habituation
Studies of the behavior of
primitive organisms go all the way back to the late 1800s, when Charles Darwin
and his son Francis proposed that in plants, the very tips of their roots (a
small region called the root apex) could act as their brains. Herbert Spencer
Jennings, an influential zoologist and early geneticist, made the same argument
in his seminal 1906 book Behavior of the Lower Organisms.
However, the notion that
single-celled organisms can learn something and retain their memory of it at
the cellular level is new and controversial. Traditionally, scientists have
directly linked the phenomenon of learning to the existence of a nervous
system. A number of people, Dussutour said, thought that her research “was a
terrible waste of time and that I would reach a dead end.”
Audrey Dussutour, a biologist who
studies animal cognition and the plasticity of organisms at France’s National
Center for Scientific Research, holds a dish of cultured slime mold. She
believes that such organisms might clarify how learning first evolved.
She
started studying the slimy blobs by putting herself “in the position of the
slime mold,” she said — wondering what it would need to learn about its
environment to survive and thrive. Slime molds crawl slowly, and they can
easily find themselves stuck in environments that are too dry, salty or acidic.
Dussutour wondered if slime molds could get used to uncomfortable conditions,
and she came up with a way to test their habituation abilities.
Habituation is not just
adaptation; it’s considered to be the simplest form of learning. It refers to
how an organism responds when it encounters the same conditions repeatedly, and
whether it can filter out a stimulus that it has realized is irrelevant. For
humans, a classic example of habituation is that we stop noticing the sensation
of our clothes against our skin moments after we put them on. We can similarly
stop noticing many unpleasant smells or background sounds, especially if they
are unchanging, when they are unimportant to our survival. For us and for other
animals, this form of learning is made possible by the networks of neurons in
our nervous systems that detect and process the stimuli and mediate our
responses. But how could habituation happen in unicellular organisms without
neurons?
Starting in 2015, Dussutour and
her team obtained samples of slime molds from colleagues at Hakodate University
in Japan and tested their ability to habituate. The researchers set up pieces
of slime mold in the lab and placed dishes of oatmeal, one of the organism’s
favorite foods, a short distance away. To reach the oatmeal, the slime molds
had to grow across gelatin bridges laced with either caffeine or quinine,
harmless but bitter chemicals that the organisms are known to avoid.
“In the first experiment, the
slime molds took 10 hours to cross the bridge and they really tried not to
touch it,” Dussutour said. After two days, the slime molds began to ignore the
bitter substance, and after six days each group stopped responding to the deterrent.
The habituation that the slime
molds had learned was specific to the substance: Slime molds that had
habituated to caffeine were still reluctant to cross a bridge containing
quinine, and vice versa. This showed that the organisms had learned to
recognize a particular stimulus and to adjust their response to it, and not to
push across bridges indiscriminately.
In experiments conducted by
Dussutour’s team, disks of yellow slime mold (at bottom) can eat plates of
oatmeal (at top) — but only if they cross gelatinous bridges (at center) laced
with noxious but harmless compounds. Here, the middle slime mold sample has
learned to disregard the chemicals, a process called habituation.
Finally,
the scientists let the slime molds rest for two days in situations where they
were exposed to neither quinine nor caffeine, and then tested them with the
noxious bridges again. “We saw that they recover — as they show avoidance
again,” Dussutour said. The slime molds had gone back to their original
behavior.
Of course, organisms can adapt to
environmental changes in ways that don’t necessarily imply learning. But
Dussutour’s work suggests that the slime molds can sometimes pick up these
behaviors through a form of communication, not just through experience.
In a follow-up study, her team showed that “naïve,”
non-habituated slime molds can directly acquire a learned behavior from
habituated ones via cell fusion.
Unlike complex multicellular
organisms, slime molds can be cut into many pieces; once they’re put back
together, they fuse and make a single giant slime mold, with veinlike tubes
filled with fast-flowing cytoplasm forming between pieces as they connect.
Dussutour cut her slime molds into more than 4,000 pieces and trained half of
them with salt — another substance that the organisms dislike, though not as
strongly as quinine and caffeine. The team fused the assorted pieces in various
combinations, mixing slime molds habituated to salt with non-habituated ones.
They then tested the new entities.
“We showed that when there was
one habituated slime mold in the entity that we were forming, the entity was
showing habituation,” she said. “So one slime mold would transfer this
habituated response to the other.” The researchers then separated the different
molds again after three hours — the time it took for all the veins of cytoplasm
to form properly — and both parts still showed habituation. The organism had
learned.
Hints of Primitive Cognition
But Dussutour wanted to push
further and see whether that habituating memory could be recalled in the long
term. So she and her team put the blobs to sleep for a year by drying them up
in a controlled manner. In March, they woke up the blobs — which found
themselves surrounded by salt. The non-habituated slime molds died, perhaps
from osmotic shock because they could not cope with how rapidly moisture leaked
out of their cells. “We lost a lot of slime molds like that,” Dussutour said.
“But habituated ones survived.” They also quickly started extending out
across their salty surroundings to hunt for food.
What that means, according to
Dussutour, who described this unpublished work at a scientific meeting in April
at the University of Bremen in Germany, is that a slime mold can learn — and it
can keep that knowledge during dormancy, despite the extensive physical and
biochemical changes in the cells that accompany that transformation. Being able
to remember where to find food is a useful skill for a slime mold to have in
the wild, because its environment can be treacherous. “It’s very good it can
habituate, otherwise it’d be stuck,” Dussutour said.
More fundamentally, she said,
this result also means that there is such a thing as “primitive cognition,” a
form of cognition that is not restricted to organisms with a brain.
Scientists have no idea what
mechanism underpins this kind of cognition. Baluška thinks that a number of
processes and molecules might be involved, and that they may vary among simple
organisms. In the case of slime molds, their cytoskeleton may form smart, complex
networks able to process sensory information. “They feed this information up to
the nuclei,” he said.
It’s not just slime molds that
may be able to learn. Researchers are investigating other nonneural organisms,
such as plants, to discover whether they can display the most basic form of
learning. For example, in 2014 Monica Gagliano and
her colleagues at the University of Western Australia and the University of
Firenze in Italy published a paper that
caused a media frenzy, on experiments with Mimosa pudicaplants. Mimosa plants
are famously sensitive to being touched or otherwise physically disturbed: They
immediately curl up their delicate leaves as a defense mechanism. Gagliano
built a mechanism that would abruptly drop the plants by about a foot without
harming them. At first, the plants would retract and curl their leaves when
they were dropped. But after a while, the plants stopped reacting — they
seemingly “learned” that no defensive response was necessary.
Slime molds are highly efficient
at exploring their environment and making use of the resources they find there.
Researchers have harnessed this ability to solve mazes and other problems under
controlled conditions.
Traditionally,
simple organisms without brains or neurons were thought to be capable of simple
stimulus-response behavior at most. Research into the behavior of protozoa such
as the slime mold Physarum polycephalum (especially the work
of Toshiyuki Nakagaki at
Hokkaido University in Japan) suggests that these seemingly simple organisms are capable of complex decision-making and
problem-solving within their environments. Nakagaki and his
colleagues have shown, for example, that slime molds are capable of solving maze problems and laying out distribution networks as
efficient as ones designed by humans (in one famous result, slime molds recreated
the Tokyo rail system).
Chris Reid and his
colleague Simon Garnier, who heads
the Swarm Lab at the New Jersey Institute of Technology, are working on the
mechanism behind how a slime mold transfers information between all of its
parts to act as a kind of collective that mimics the capabilities of a brain
full of neurons. Each tiny part of the slime mold contracts and expands over
the course of about one minute, but the contraction rate is linked to the
quality of the local environment. Attractive stimuli cause faster pulsations,
while negative stimuli cause the pulsations to slow. Each pulsing part also
influences the pulsing frequency of its neighbors, not unlike the way the
firing rates of linked neurons influence one another. Using computer vision
techniques and experiments that might be likened to a slime mold version of an
MRI brain scan, the researchers are examining how the slime mold uses this
mechanism to transfer information around its giant unicellular body and make
complex decisions between conflicting stimuli.
Fighting to Keep Brains Special
But some mainstream biologists
and neuroscientists are critical of the results. “Neuroscientists are objecting
to the ‘devaluing’ of the specialness of the brain,” said Michael Levin, a biologist at Tufts University.
“Brains are great, but we have to remember where they came from. Neurons
evolved from nonneural cells, they did not magically appear.”
Some biologists also object “to
the idea that cells can have goals, memories and so on, because it sounds like
magic,” he added. But we have to remember, he said, that work on control
theory, cybernetics, artificial intelligence and machine learning over the last
century or so has shown that mechanistic systems can have goals and make
decisions. “Computer science long ago learned that information processing is
substrate-independent,” Levin said. “It’s not about what you’re made of, it’s
about how you compute.”
It all depends on how one defines
learning, according to John Smythies, the director of the Laboratory for
Integrative Neuroscience at the University of California, San Diego. He is not
persuaded that Dussutour’s experiment with slime molds staying habituated to
salt after extended dormancy shows much. “‘Learning’ implies behavior and dying
is not that!” he said.
To Fred Kaijzer, a cognitive scientist at the
University of Groningen in the Netherlands, the question of whether these
interesting behaviors show that slime molds can learn is similar to the debate
over whether Pluto is a planet: The answer depends as much on how the concept
of learning is cast as on the empirical evidence. Still, he said, “I do not see
any clear-cut scientific reasons for denying the option that nonneural
organisms can actually learn”.
Baluška said that many
researchers also fiercely disagree about whether plants can have memories,
learning and cognition. Plants are still considered to be “zombielike automata
rather than full-blown living organisms,” he said.
But the common perception is
slowly changing. “In plants, we started the plant neurobiology initiative in
2005, and although still not accepted by the mainstream, we already changed it
so much that terms like plant signaling, communication and behavior are
more or less accepted now,” he said.
The debate is arguably not a war
about the science, but about words. “Most neuroscientists I have talked to
about slime mold intelligence are quite happy to accept that the experiments
are valid and show similar functional outcomes to the same experiments
performed on animals with brains,” Reid said. What they seem to take issue with
is the use of terms traditionally reserved for psychology and neuroscience and
almost universally associated with brains, such as learning, memory and
intelligence. “Slime mold researchers insist that functionally equivalent
behavior observed in the slime mold should use the same descriptive terms as
for brained animals, while classical neuroscientists insist that the very
definition of learning and intelligence requires a neuron-
Baluška said that as a result,
it’s not that easy to get grants for primitive-cognition studies. “The most
important issue is that grant agencies and funding bodies will start to support
such project proposals. Until now, the mainstream science, despite a few
exceptions, is rather reluctant in this respect, which is a real pity.”
To gain mainstream recognition,
researchers of primitive cognition will have to demonstrate habituation to a
broad range of stimuli, and — most importantly — determine the exact mechanisms
by which habituation is achieved and how it can be transferred between single
cells, Reid said. “This mechanism must be quite different to that observed in
brains, but the similarities in functional outcomes make the comparison
extremely interesting.”